The Neuroscience of Free Will: A Q&A with Robyn Repko Waller

We discuss blending philosophy and neuroscience in unraveling the mysteries of free will

Who are you and how did you become interested in free will?

I am an Assistant Professor of Philosophy at Iona College where I also serve as a faculty member for the Iona Neuroscience program. I have previously worked in the Scientific and Philosophical Studies of Mind program at Franklin and Marshall College as well as previous appointments as a Lecturer at Kingโ€™s College London and the University of Alabama.

My recent and forthcoming publications focus on issues of autonomy in terms of philosophical accounts of free will as well as how it intersects with neuroscience and psychiatry. One of the main questions I investigate is what neuroscience can tell us about meaningful agency (see here for my recent review of the topic as part of an extended review of research on agency, freedom, and responsibility for the John Templeton Foundation).

I became interested in free will via an interdisciplinary route. As an undergraduate at Grinnell College, I majored in psychology with a strong emphasis on experimental psychology and clinical psychology. During my senior year at Grinnell, I realized that I was fascinated by the theoretical issues operating in the background of the psychological studies that we read and conducted, especially issues of how the mind is related to the brain, prospects for the scientific study of consciousness, and how humans as agents fit into a natural picture of the world. So I followed these interests to the study of the philosophy of psychology and eventually found my way to the perfect fusion of these topics: the neuroscience of free will.

What is free will?

Free will seems to be a familiar feature of our everyday lives โ€” most of us believe that (at least at times) what we do is up to us to some extent.

For instance, I freely decided to take my job or that I am acting freely when I decide to go for a run this afternoon. Free will is not just that I move about in the world to achieve a goal, but that I exercise meaningful control over what I decide to do. My decisions and actions are up to me in the sense that they are mine โ€” a product of my values, desires, beliefs, and intentions. I decided to take this job because I valued the institutionโ€™s mission or I believed that this job would be enriching or a good fit for me.

Related: Neuroscience of Anxiety in the Bright Brain

True Love Is Built On Free Will

Correspondingly, it seems to me that at least at times I could have decided to and done something else than what I did. I decided to go for a run this afternoon, but no one made me and I wasnโ€™t subject to any compulsion; I could have gone for a coffee instead, at least it seems to me.

Philosophers take these starting points and work to construct plausible accounts of free will. Broadly speaking, there is a lot of disagreement as to the right view of free will, but most philosophers believe that a person has free will if they have the ability to act freely, and that this kind of control is linked to whether it would be appropriate to hold that person responsible (e.g., blame or praise them) for what they do. For instance, we donโ€™t typically hold people responsible for what they do if they were acting under severe threat or inner compulsion.

How do neuroscientists study free will?

There are plenty of sensational claims about the brain science of free will out there and lots of back and forth about whether or not science disproves free will (e.g., โ€œMy brain made me do itโ€).

Related:ย The Neuroscience of Drumming

Given the strong link between free will and systems of moral and legal responsibility, like punishment, the stakes are high not just for our conception of human nature, but also for our everyday practices that matter.

The current neuroscience of free will traces its lineage back to an influential experiment by Benjamin Libet and his colleagues. The majority of our actions begin with bodily movements, and most of us think that when we decide to move (e.g., decide to pick up my cup of tea), first I, the agent or person, decides and then I hand off control, so to speak, to the brain circuits for motor control to execute the action.

It was known since the 1960s from work by Kornhuber and Deecke that there is a slow buildup of negative brain activity in the supplementary motor area (SMA) and pre-SMA measurable by electroencephalography (EEG) just prior to voluntary (i.e., movement initiated by the participant) bodily movement.

This brain activity, called the readiness potential (RP), was taken to be neural preparation to move for spontaneous movements and starts about a half-second before the time of the movement (here).

So Libet and his fellow researchers ask when does the agent appears in relation to the RP? The agentโ€™s decision has to be something measurable in the lab, so Libet asked participants to make movements (of the finger or wrist) at a time of their choosing and then report after the fact when they were first aware of their decision or urge to move using a modified clock (termed โ€˜W timeโ€™).

Libet found, contra the commonsense expectation, that the average reported time of first awareness of the decision to move, W-time, occurred almost a third of a second after the start of the RP. So Libet (and select others since) concluded that the RP is the brainโ€™s unconscious decision to move with the agentโ€™s decision occurring later (here).

Related:ย The Science Behind Mass Panic and How To Stay Sane During Coronavirus Outbreak

Libet took this as evidence that the conscious agent or self doesnโ€™t initiate, or kick off, preparation to act, the unconscious brain does. He argued that this result is representative of how all of our voluntary movements are produced, and, if so, then the agentโ€™s conscious decision to act doesnโ€™t initiate the process leading to movement. But if the agent doesnโ€™t play this initiating role in acting, how can it be up to me how I act?

These results have worried a lot of folks and inspired a booming research enterprise in cognitive neuroscience and philosophy. One shouldnโ€™t jump to the depressing conclusion, though, that we donโ€™t act freely or donโ€™t really deserve any of the moral reactions others have to our actions; there is a healthy discussion on how the original Libet results can be interpreted as consistent with that picture of us humans as self-governing and free and moral persons.

W-time is taken to indicate the moment of awareness of a decision. Can we capture โ€œmoments of conscious awarenessโ€ scientifically?

Since the initial publication of Libet and colleaguesโ€™ study, worries about whether we could measure the time of conscious awareness have been voiced. After all, we are talking here about the timeframe of milliseconds.

What does the study reveal?ย 

In these studies, all of the events measured prior to movement in the lab are happening within one second before the participant wiggles a finger or hand (now button presses are the preferred movement). Libet argued that W-time within a reasonable range was reliable since we can see how accurately participants in the lab estimate the time of other events, such as skin shocks. The reliability of W-time has recently been challenged yet again with a new study that concludes that depending on the order in which participants complete certain tasks in the experiment, W-time can be strikingly different (i.e., there is an order effect; see here).

Other researchers are currently exploring alternative ways to measure a decision to move in the lab, including work by Parรฉs-Pujolrร s and co-authors, who have been using an online (i.e., pre-movement) measure of the agentโ€™s awareness of a decision to move (here).

Related:ย Your Partner Can Control Your Brain, Science Explains

In these studies, participants watch a continuous stream of letters on a computer while spontaneously pressing a button. Every now and then, though, the letters change color. When this happens participants are told to press the button just then if they were already aware of their preparing to press the button soon. These kinds of online measures of awareness may yet prove to be more reliable ways of getting at whether people have conscious intentions to act in the lab.

Whatโ€™s the latest work on the neuroscience of free will?

Two of the hottest topics seem to be, first, what exactly the RP, that negative build-up of brain activity pre-movement, really signifies and, second, how we can make our voluntary actions in the lab more ecologically valid.

As to the first, the past decade has seen researchers investigating if we have evidence that the RP really does stand for a decision to move or, alternatively, if the RP just is the brainโ€™s being biased to move in some way (say, left, instead of right) without the commitment to do so.

Related:ย Science Confirms: Stress Causes Premature Hair Greying

Others test the possibility that the RP isnโ€™t really movement specific activity at all (e.g., general cognitive preparation to perform a task voluntarily). Others, such as Schurger and colleagues, have argued via empirical studies that the RP is the neural signature that we pick up when are actions are generated by neural noise crossing some threshold (here). That possibility would be alarming as then our actions, which we take to be undertaken by me for reasons, may really just be the passive result of fluctuating brain activity.

As to the second hot issue, researchers are now attempting to design tasks in the lab that are closer to the kind of decisions and activities that we engage in daily. Libet argued that a simple movement like a wrist flexor button press could stand-in for the more complex actions, as the RP has been shown to occur prior to more complex movements in the lab. Hence we could give a unified explanation of the timing of events involving practical decisions and bodily movements.

But many, myself included, have voiced concern that when to press a button or whether to press a left or right button, just isnโ€™t the right kind of action to stake a claim that we as agents donโ€™t initiate our actions via our conscious intentions to act.

Hence, some of the ongoing work involves making the choice of which button to press or when to press it meaningful via rewards or penalties for skipping ahead or value-laden options, such as charity donations.*

And, of course, there are plenty of neuroimaging tools at the disposal of cognitive neuroscientists. Some of the most interesting replications and extensions of the Libet findings have been done using singe-cell recording and fMRI among other technologies (see here and here, respectively).

Related:ย Science Confirms That Cats and Dogs Can See โ€œSpiritsโ€ and Frequencies That Humans Canโ€™t

In fact, the neuroscience of free will has been and currently is the focus of some major research grants, such as the Big Questions in Free Will project (2010-2014, Principal Investigator Dr. Alfred Mele) and the Consciousness and Free Will project (2019-, a collaboration across 17 PIs), each of which involves philosophers and numerous neuroscientific labs worldwide. From these grants, I think we should expect further clarity on whatโ€™s going on under the hood, so to speak, when we decide what to do and act voluntarily.

Are there any other results in neuroscience that tell us something intriguing about our agential control?

Yes, one of the aspects of our lives that seems the most undeniable is that we really do experience ourselves as in control of our movements and their effects in the world. There is a large body of work in cognitive neuroscience which focuses on this sense of agency via research on whatโ€™s been termed intentional binding.

Basically, if you ask participants in clever experimental set-ups to judge whether some event (e.g., icon moving on a computer screen) was the outcome of their agency or someone elseโ€™s (i.e., โ€œI did thatโ€ judgments), participants tend to misjudge an outcome to be a result of their own agency if it is a positive one and misjudge an outcome to be the result of anotherโ€™s agency if it is a negative one.

That is, there is a self-serving bias to explicit sense of agency judgments (For interesting results in this regard see Wegner and Wheatleyโ€™s 1999 paper here and other earlier work in psychology on attribution theory).

Cognitive neuroscientists have found a methodology to study our sense that we are in control of our actions and actional outcomes without surveying participantsโ€™ explicit โ€œI did thatโ€ judgments. Instead, experimenters asked participants to judge the time of various events, including their movements (e.g., a button press) and the sensory outcomes of those movements (e.g., a beep following the button press).

Related: How Cognitive Distortions Harm Us

What researchers have found is that if you voluntarily press a button and hear a tone as a consequence, you are going to judge that the time of the movement and the time of the tone are much closer together in perceived space than if you are caused to move (via neural stimulation) and hear a tone as a consequence.

Related:ย Dogs Can Sense Bad People according to science

In other words, the perceived time of the action and the tone โ€œbind togetherโ€ in perceptual space when you act voluntarily as opposed to when you are caused to move or simply judge the time of events without acting (here). Whatโ€™s intriguing about this research on agency, then, is that our perceptual judgments about the world seem to distinguish when we act from when something is done to us. Research work on intentional binding has tackled more ecologically valid issues of sense of agency when acting under emotional distress, due to coercion, and in the face of options.*

* Neuroscientists working on more representative kinds of decisions and/or sense of agency in more ecologically valid contexts include researchers in the UCL Action and Body Lab at University College London and The Brain Institute at Chapman University, among others.


Written by Scott Barry Kaufman
Originally appeared in Scientific American

Neuroscience of Free Will Everything You Need To Know pin

— Share —

,

Leave a Reply

Your email address will not be published. Required fields are marked *

Up Next

Is Your Depression Causing Anger? 4 Crucial Reasons to Address It

Do you find yourself caught in an emotional tug-of-war? Is your depression causing anger? If every little frustration feels like it could explode into rage and snapping at loved ones for no reason has become a common habit, learn the ways to help yourself with depression and anger.

(adsbygoogle = window.adsbygoogle || []).push({});

As an effect of depression, anger is quite common because

Up Next

Maladaptive Daydreaming: 5 Warning Signs That Your Inner World Is Distracting You

If you frequently find yourself lost in your thoughts, consider learning about maladaptive daydreaming disorder to help yourself from being trapped in your imagination!

(adsbygoogle = window.adsbygoogle || []).push({});

Taking a few moments to daydream can help a person take a break from reality and think more creatively or even solve problems. But when it goes to

Up Next

8 Everyday Habits That Are Damaging Your Brain (And What To Do Instead)

We all do things we know we shouldnโ€™t- that we know are unhealthy. Skipping lunch because you woke up late for work, or staying up past bedtime binging on the new show on Netflix (because who can resist, right?). These might seem harmless, but some of these are habits that are damaging your brain.

(adsbygoogle = window.adsbygoogle || []).push({});

Our brain is the most vital organ of our

Up Next

Why Itโ€™s So Hard to Admit Youโ€™re Wrong: 7 Surprising Psychological Barriers You Didnโ€™t Know About

Have you ever found yourself in the middle of a heated debate, feeling that gnawing sensation that you might be wrong, but donโ€™t want to admit it? Or maybe you are watching someone double down on their stance, even when all evidence points to the contrary, and wondering, โ€œWhy canโ€™t they just say theyโ€™re wrong?โ€ Itโ€™s a common scenario that plays out in classrooms, workplaces, and even family dinners, leaving many of us puzzled about why it is so hard to admit to being wrong.

Up Next

Are Music and Personality Traits Connected? Exposing Secrets of Your Music Taste

Whether youโ€™re a die-hard rock fan, a classical music lover, or a pop enthusiast, your playlist might just be the key to understanding who you truly are.ย Music lovers, prepare to have your minds blown! Letโ€™s dive deep into the fascinating connection between music and personality.

(adsbygoogle = window.adsbygoogle || []).push({});

Get ready to discover how your favorite tunes might be

Up Next

3 Hacks To Stop Negative Thinking: It Worked For Me!

As someone who struggled to recognize negative thinking in myself, it took some time to understand I was doing it as a self-defense mechanism. I felt like I โ€œhad toโ€ think negatively to keep myself safe. After a series of negative consequences, I discovered three easy steps to help stop negative thinking.

(adsbygoogle = window.adsbygoogle || []).push({});

Negativity is not just in your

Up Next

9 Habits Of Highly Intelligent People That Sets Them Apart

Understanding the habits of highly intelligent people can offer useful clues about what sets them apart from the rest of us.ย 

(adsbygoogle = window.adsbygoogle || []).push({});

Intelligence is multi-dimensional, but it is your habits that show how well you can use it in lif